|
Violent video game engagement is not associated with adolescents' aggressive behaviour
|
|
|
| 22nd August 2020
|
|
| See paper from royalsocietypublishing.org |
Violent video game engagement is not associated with adolescents' aggressive behaviour: evidence from a registered report By Andrew K. Przybylski and Netta Weinstein Abstract
In this study, we investigated the extent to which adolescents who spend time playing violent video games exhibit higher levels of aggressive behaviour when compared with those who do not. A large sample of British adolescent
participants (n = 1004) aged 14 and 15 years and an equal number of their carers were interviewed. Young people provided reports of their recent gaming experiences. Further, the violent contents of these games were coded using
official EU and US ratings, and carers provided evaluations of their adolescents' aggressive behaviours in the past month. Following a preregistered analysis plan, multiple regression analyses tested the hypothesis that recent
violent game play is linearly and positively related to carer assessments of aggressive behaviour. Results did not support this prediction, nor did they support the idea that the relationship between these factors follows a
nonlinear parabolic function. There was no evidence for a critical tipping point relating violent game engagement to aggressive behaviour. Sensitivity and exploratory analyses indicated these null effects extended across multiple operationalizations of
violent game engagement and when the focus was on another behavioural outcome, namely, prosocial behaviour. The discussion presents an interpretation of this pattern of effects in terms of both the ongoing scientific and policy
debates around violent video games, and emerging standards for robust evidence-based policy concerning young people's technology use.
|
|
Researchers claim that 'Children's Violent Video Game Play Associated with Increased Physical Aggressive Behavior'
|
|
|
| 29th April 2019
|
|
| See press release from dartmouth.edu
See paper from pnas.org |
Children's Violent Video Game Play Associated with Increased Physical Aggressive Behavior Dartmouth Analysis of Multiple Studies Demonstrates How Effect Varies Across Ethnicity Violent video game
play by adolescents is associated with increases in physical aggression over time, according to a Dartmouth meta-analysis published in the Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences (PNAS) . Although most researchers on the subject agree that playing violent video games appears to increase physical aggression, a vocal minority continues to dispute this. To examine issues
raised by the counterclaims on this topic, Dartmouth researchers conducted a meta-analysis of 24 studies from around the world from 2010 to 2017 with over 17,000 participants, ages nine to 19 years-old. The studies all examined how violent video game
play affected changes in real-world physical aggression over time, ranging from three months to four years. Examples of physical aggression included incidents such as hitting someone or being sent to the principal's office for fighting, and were based on
self-reports by children, parents, teachers and peers. Dartmouth's study examined three specific critiques of the literature on video game play and aggression:
To address claims that previous meta-analyses overestimate the association of violent video game play and aggression because they include "non-serious" measures of aggression, this meta-analysis was limited to studies
that measured reports of overt, physical aggression over time. Despite this more stringent criterion, findings supported the hypothesis that playing violent games is associated with subsequent increases in physical aggression. -
To investigate claims that effects are often inflated because many studies do not take into account other variables predictive of aggressive behavior, Dartmouth researchers compared analyses that included or did not include
information on such variables and found that taking these data into account had only a minor effect on the size of the observed relation between violent video game play and aggression. To evaluate claims that the estimated
effect of violent game play on aggression is inflated because of a bias against publishing studies that fail to find a relation of violent game play and aggression, Dartmouth researchers conducted a variety of different tests and found no evidence of
publication bias.
In addition to providing evidence that violent video game play is associated with increased aggression over time, the study also reports that this effect appears to be significantly different for various ethnic groups: the largest
effect was observed among white participants, with some effect noted among Asians and no effect observed among Hispanics. Although speculative, the authors suggest that this effect may reflect a greater emphasis on maintaining empathy toward victims of
aggression among Eastern and Hispanic cultures in contrast to an emphasis on "rugged individualism" in Western cultures. Lead author Jay G. Hull , the Dartmouth Professor of Psychological and Brain Sciences, said:
Although no single research project is definitive, our research aims to provide the most current and compelling responses to key criticisms on this topic. Based on our findings, we feel it is clear that violent video game
play is associated with subsequent increases in physical aggression.
Co-author James D. Sargent, the Scott M. and Lisa G. Stuart Professor of Pediatric Oncology said: The most
notable critic of the violent video game aggression literature conducted studies in primarily Hispanic populations and found no evidence of this association. If all of my studies showed null findings, I too, would be skeptical. I hope our findings prompt
skeptics to reevaluate their position, especially since some of our other research indicates that violent video game play may increase deviance with implications for multiple risk behaviors.
|
|
Violent video games found not to be associated with adolescent aggression
|
|
|
| 18th February 2019
|
|
| See press release from
oii.ox.ac.uk See full paper from royalsocietypublishing.org |
Researchers at the Oxford Internet Institute, University of Oxford, have found no relationship between aggressive behaviour in teenagers and the amount of time spent playing violent video games. The study used nationally representative data from
British teens and their parents alongside official E.U. and US ratings of game violence. The findings were published in Royal Society Open Science. The idea that violent video games drive real-world aggression is a popular one,
but it hasn't tested very well over time, says lead researcher Professor Andrew Przybylski, Director of Research at the Oxford Internet Institute. Despite interest in the topic by parents and policy-makers, the research has not demonstrated that there is
cause for concern. The study is one of the most definitive to date, using a combination of subjective and objective data to measure teen aggression and violence in games. Unlike previous research on the topic, which relied heavily
on self-reported data from teenagers, the study used information from parents and carers to judge the level of aggressive behaviour in their children. Additionally, the content of the video games was classified using the official Pan European Game
Information (EU) and Entertainment Software Rating Board (US) rating system, rather than only player's perceptions of the amount of violence in the game. Our findings suggest that researcher biases might have influenced previous
studies on this topic, and have distorted our understanding of the effects of video games, says co-author Dr Netta Weinstein from Cardiff University. An important step taken in this study was preregistration, where the researchers publically registered
their hypothesis, methods and analysis technique prior to beginning the research. Part of the problem in technology research is that there are many ways to analyse the same data, which will produce different results. A
cherry-picked result can add undue weight to the moral panic surrounding video games. The registered study approach is a safe-guard against this, says Przybylski. While no correlation was found between playing video games and
aggressive behaviour in teenagers, the researchers emphasize that this does not mean that some mechanics and situations in gaming do not provoke angry feelings or reactions in players. Anecdotally, you do see things such as trash-talking, competitiveness
and trolling in gaming communities that could qualify as antisocial behaviour, says Przybylski. This would be an interesting avenue for further research. Researchers should use the registered study approach to investigate other
media effects phenomena. There are a lot of ideas out there like 'social media drives depression and technology addiction that lowers quality of life that simply have no supporting evidence. These topics and others that drive technological anxieties
should be studied more rigorously 203 society needs solid evidence in order to make appropriate policy decisions.' The data was drawn from a nationally representative sample of British 14- and 15-year olds, and the same number of
their carers (totalling 2,008 subjects). Teenagers completed questions on their personality and gaming behaviour over the past month, while carers completed questions on their child's recent aggressive behaviours using the widely-used Strengths and
Difficulties Questionnaire. The violent content in the games played were coded based on their rating in the official Pan European Game Information (PEGI; EU) and Entertainment Software Rating Board (ESRB; US) rating system, as well as player's subjective
rating. The findings of the study were derived from a study following the Registered Reports Protocol; the study's sampling plan and statistical approach were evaluated before the data were collected. Multiple linear regression modelling tested whether
the relations between regular violent video game play (coded by researchers) and adolescents' aggressive and helping behaviours (judged by parents) were positive, negative, linear, or parabolic.
|
|
|
|
|
|
23rd October 2016
|
|
|
A more nuanced look at the relationship between violence and games. By Jesse Marczyk See article
from psychologytoday.com |
|
|
|
|
| 10th
March 2016
|
|
|
Nobody knows...and this is why See article from theguardian.com |
|
American Psychological Association review scientific papers on computer games and violence
|
|
|
| 6th September
2015
|
|
| See press release from apa.org |
Violent video game play is linked to increased aggression in players but insufficient evidence exists about whether the link extends to criminal violence or delinquency, according to a new American Psychological Association task force report. Mark
Appelbaum, the task force chair, commented in the review: The research demonstrates a consistent relation between violent video game use and increases in aggressive behavior, aggressive cognitions and aggressive
affect, and decreases in prosocial behavior, empathy and sensitivity to aggression Scientists have investigated the use of violent video games for more than two decades but to date, there is very limited research addressing
whether violent video games cause people to commit acts of criminal violence. However, the link between violence in video games and increased aggression in players is one of the most studied and best established in the field. No
single risk factor consistently leads a person to act aggressively or violently, the report states. Rather, it is the accumulation of risk factors that tends to lead to aggressive or violent behavior. The research reviewed here demonstrates that violent
video game use is one such risk factor.
In light of the task force's conclusions, APA has called on the industry to design video games that include increased parental control over the amount of violence the games contain. APA's
Council of Representatives adopted a resolution encouraging the Entertainment Software Rating Board to refine its video game rating system to reflect the levels and characteristics of violence in games, in addition to the current global ratings. In addition, the resolution urges developers to design games that are appropriate to users' age and psychological development, and voices APA's support for more research to address gaps in the knowledge about the effects of violent video game use.
The task force conducted a comprehensive review of the research literature published between 2005 and 2013 focused on violent video game use. This included four meta-analyses that reviewed more than 150 research reports published before 2009. Task
force members then conducted both a systematic evidence review and a quantitative review of the literature published between 2009 and 2013. (A systematic evidence review synthesizes all empirical evidence that meets pre-specified criteria to answer
specific research questions) This resulted in 170 articles, 31 of which met all of the most stringent screening criteria. Update: ESRB responds 18th September 2015.See article from gamepolitics.com
In addition to the report described above, the APA released a declaration: A Resolution on Violent Video Games - that strongly encourages the Entertainment Software Rating Board to refine the ESRB rating system
specifically to reflect the levels and characteristics of violence in games in addition to the current global ratings .
While the ESRB said that it has had an open dialogue with the APA - and will continue to do so, it also said that
it doesn't need to make changes to the ratings system. It cited an 8-year-old FTC report on the reliability of the ratings system (compared to other entertainment industry ratings systems) and a Hart Research poll that found parents were familiar with
the ESRB. |
|
|
|
|
|
12th May 2015
|
|
|
Writer shows that porn research rewires the brain and is linked to cerebral dysfunction. By Damian Thompson See
article from blogs.spectator.co.uk |
|
Er.. no. Another research paper finds no correlation
|
|
|
|
9th November 2014
|
|
| See press release from
eurekalert.org |
Does Media Violence Predict Societal Violence? It Depends on What You Look at and When, by Christopher Ferguson; Journal of Communication Since the 1920s, scholars and politicians have blamed violence in movies and
other media as a contributing factor to rising violence in society. Recently the responses to mass shootings in Aurora, CO and at Sandy Hook Elementary followed this theme as media consumption came into the equation. But can consumption of violent media
really be a factor in real-world violence? A recent study published in the Journal of Communication by a researcher at Stetson University found that there were no associations between media violence consumption in society and societal violence.
Christopher Ferguson (Stetson University) published his findings in the Journal of Communication. Ferguson conducted two studies that raised the question if whether the incidence of violence in media correlates with actual violence
rates in society. The first study looked at movie violence and homicide rates between 1920 and 2005. The second study looked at videogame violence consumption and its relationship to youth violence rates from 1996-2011. He found that societal consumption
of media violence is not predictive of increased violence rates in society. For the first study, independent raters evaluated the frequency and graphicness of violence in popular movies from 1920-2005. These were correlated to
homicide rates for the same years. Overall, movie violence and homicide rates were not correlated. However, during the mid-20th century, movie violence and homicide rates did appear to correlate slightly, which may have led some to believe a larger trend
was at play. That correlation reversed after 1990 so that movie violence became correlated with fewer homicides. Prior to the 1940s, movie violence was similarly related to fewer homicides, not more. In the second study on video
game violence, the Entertainment Software Ratings Board (ESRB) ratings were used to estimate the violent content of the most popular video games for the years 1996-2011. These estimates of societal video game violence consumption were correlated against
federal data on youth violence rates during the same years. Violent video game consumption was strongly correlated with declines in youth violence. However, it was concluded that such a correlation is most likely due to chance and does not indicate video
games caused the decline in youth violence. Previous studies have focused on laboratory experiments and aggression as a response to movie and videogame violence, but this does not match well with real-life exposure. Other studies
have indicated that, in the short term, the release of violent movies or video games is associated with declines in societal violence. However, no one has examined these trends long-term. Some scholars have argued that movies are becoming more violent,
but none have examined whether this phenomenon is a concern for society. This study is the first to suggest that movie violence and video game violence consumption probably are increasing over time, but that there is little evidence that this has caused
a problem for society. Society has a limited amount of resources and attention to devote to the problem of reducing crime. There is a risk that identifying the wrong problem, such as media violence, may distract society from
more pressing concerns such as poverty, education and vocational disparities and mental health, Ferguson said. This research may help society focus on issues that really matter and avoid devoting unnecessary resources to the pursuit of moral
agendas with little practical value.
|
|
Media blame game stories on the decrease even though there has been an increase in the realism of computer game violence
|
|
|
| 6th April 2014
|
|
| See
nes release from
munews.missouri.edu |
Members of the media and others often have attributed violence in video games as a potential cause of social ills, such as increased levels of teen violence and school shootings. Now, a University of Missouri researcher has found that media acceptance of
video game violence has increased as video game technology has improved over time. Greg Perreault, a doctoral student at the MU School of Journalism, examined the coverage of violent video games throughout the 1990s by GamePro Magazine, the most popular
video game news magazine during that time period. Perreault found that journalists from GamePro expressed a considerable amount of concern about the level of violence in the game software companies were creating in the early 1990s, when video game design
was limited by technology. Perreault said: Early in the '90s, when video games were still a relatively new medium, journalists expressed quite a bit of concern about the level of violence in many of the games,. It is
interesting because the simulated violence in these games was so mild relative to modern-day games.
As technology improved throughout 1990s, new gaming systems such as the Nintendo 64 and Sony Playstation were
released, along with the capacity for higher levels of graphic violence. Perreault found that despite this increase, the levels of concern about violence from GamePro journalists decreased. Perreault said: As
technology improved and the animations became more and more life-like, game creators had increased capability to design more graphic violence, including blood and gore. Despite this increasing amount of violence, journalists seemed to be less and less
bothered by the blood and guts. This is important to note because journalism often mirrors the culture of the audience it serves. As technology improved, the entire gaming community became more and more comfortable with the levels of violence that were
simultaneously increasing in video games. In a sense, the gaming community grew up. They aged from children using video games as toys to adolescents and adults using them as recreational devices. It appears that journalists reflected this trend in their
writing.
Perreault says the video game rating system is another example of this trend. He says when the rating system first was created, gaming journalists opposed it; however, as games become more and more violent,
the rating system is used continually as a defense against outside criticism: As more and more parents and outside sources criticize violent games, gamers and gaming journalists point to the rating system and say that
parents should not allow their kids to play violent games with explicit ratings. Ultimately, the trend in violent games is a reflection of what interests our society. Similar trends can be found in the increased number of 'R' rated movies as well as the
popularity of gangster rap and other violent music. Video games are just another way our culture is expressing itself.
Perreault will present his research at the International Communication Association conference in
Seattle this May.
|
|
|
|
|
| 17th March 2014
|
|
|
A new study from Canada's Brock University has found that playing violent video games for significant lengths of time can hold back the moral maturity of teenagers. See
article from policymic.com |
|
TV and computer games cleared of causing behavioural problems by University of Glasgow research
|
|
|
| 26th December 2013
|
|
| From gamepolitics.com See research paper
from adc.bmj.com |
A recently released University of Glasgow study that takes the data from a survey of 11,000 children born between 2000 and 2002 comes to the conclusion that playing video games - even at a young age - does not lead to behavioral problems. The authors
of the study aimed to examine both television and video games screen time, in the hopes of finding connections with attention disorders, anger issues, and other problems that might be connected to both. Researchers wondered aloud if games may have
more powerful effects due to active user engagement, identification with characters and repeated rehearsal and reinforcement. But their research went in another direction. They learned that exposure to video games had no effect on behavior,
attention or emotional issues, and that watching three or more hours of television starting at age 5 did lead to a small increase in behavioral problems in youngsters between 5 and 7. Television and video games did not lead to attentional or emotional
problems and there seemed to be no difference between boys and girls in the survey results. The survey relied heavily on parents reporting average screen time and later behavioral problems, but the size of the research pool -- more than 13,000
families -- left researchers confident their results were solid. Researchers also said they modified the results to take into account various parenting approaches and socio-economic differences. |
|
Failure to Demonstrate That Playing Violent Video Games Diminishes Prosocial Behavior. By Tear MJ and Nielsen M
|
|
|
| 27th August 2013
|
|
| See full paper from
plosone.org |
Abstract Background Past research has found that playing a classic prosocial video game resulted in heightened prosocial behavior when compared to a control group, whereas playing a
classic violent video game had no effect. Given purported links between violent video games and poor social behavior, this result is surprising. Here our aim was to assess whether this finding may be due to the specific games used. That is, modern games
are experienced differently from classic games (more immersion in virtual environments, more connection with characters, etc.) and it may be that playing violent video games impacts prosocial behavior only when contemporary versions are used.
Methods and Findings Experiments 1 and 2 explored the effects of playing contemporary violent, non-violent, and prosocial video games on prosocial behavior, as measured by the pen-drop task. We found
that slight contextual changes in the delivery of the pen-drop task led to different rates of helping but that the type of game played had little effect. Experiment 3 explored this further by using classic games. Again, we found no effect.
Conclusions We failed to find evidence that playing video games affects prosocial behavior. Research on the effects of video game play is of significant public interest. It is therefore important that
speculation be rigorously tested and findings replicated. Here we fail to substantiate conjecture that playing contemporary violent video games will lead to diminished prosocial behavior.
|
|
Research finds that genetics and social issues predict adult criminality, not games playing
|
|
|
| 28th June 2013
|
|
| See article from gamepolitics.com See
Genetic, Maternal, School, Intelligence, and Media Use Predictors of Adult Criminality [pdf] from tamiu.edu
|
A recent study by Texas A&M International University chair and associate professor, psychology Dr. Christopher J. Ferguson shows that childhood media consumption is not a predictor for future adult criminal behavior. The long-held (and as of
yet unproven) argument has been that violent video games or other violent media have a direct causation to violent crimes like school shootings. But according to a new study from TAMIU, genetics, environment, the lack of maternal nurturing, and a number
of other factors combined are better predictors of adult criminality. The TAMIU study used data from a National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health, which included a representative sample of U.S. adolescents. Dr. Ferguson said:
We basically find that genetics and some social issues combine to predict later adult arrests. Despite ongoing concerns about media influences, media exposure does not seem to function as a risk factor for adult criminality.
Genetics alone don't seem to trigger criminal behavior, but in combination with harsh upbringing, you can see negative outcomes. In our sample, experiencing maternal warmth seemed to reduce the impact of genetics on adult criminality.
Researchers also noted that other factors such as family environment, peers and socioeconomic status can also be predictors of adult criminality. The research also found that being exposed to maternal affection may have the
potential to decrease criminal behavior in individuals who might otherwise be at risk. Ferguson concluded: People may object morally to some of the content that exists in the media, but the question is whether
the media can predict criminal behavior. The answer seems to be no.
|
|
Study finds that teenagers playing Manhunt for long periods leads to a lack of sleep
|
|
|
| 11th May 2013
|
|
| See article from
psychosomaticmedicine.org
|
The Effect of Violent and Nonviolent Video Games on Heart Rate Variability, Sleep, and Emotions in Adolescents With Different Violent Gaming Habits By Malena Ivarsson, BA, Martin Anderson, MD, Torbjorn Akerstedt, PhD
and Frank Lindblad, MD Abstract Objective To study cardiac, sleep-related, and emotional reactions to playing violent games versus nonviolent video games in adolescents with different gaming
habits. Methods Thirty boys (aged 13--16 years), half of them low-exposed (1 hour/day) and half high-exposed (3 hour/day) to violent games, played a violent games/nonviolent video games for 2 hours during two different evenings in
their homes. Heart rate and heart rate variability were registered from before start until next morning. A questionnaire about emotional reactions was administered after gaming sessions and a sleep diary on the following mornings.
Results During sleep, there were significant interaction effects between group and gaming condition for heart rate. There was also a significant interaction for sleep quality, and sadness after playing. Conclusions Different
combinations of the extent of previous violent games and experimental exposure to a violent games or an nonviolent video games are associated with different reaction patterns---physiologically, emotionally, and sleep related. Desensitizing effects or
selection bias stand out as possible explanations.
|
|
At the end of the day violent crime has gone down whilst violent gaming has gone up
|
|
|
|
22nd March 2013
|
|
| See abstract from
papers.ssrn.com See paper [pdf] from
papers.ssrn.com
|
Abstract: Video games are an increasingly popular leisure activity. As many of best-selling games contain hyper-realistic violence, many researchers and policymakers have concluded that violent games cause violent behaviors. Evidence on a causal effect of violent games on violence is usually based on laboratory experiments finding violent games increase aggression. Before drawing policy conclusions about the effect of violent games on actual behavior, these experimental studies should be subjected to tests of external validity.
Our study uses a quasi-experimental methodology to identify the short and medium run effects of violent game sales on violent crime using time variation in retail unit sales data of the top 50 selling video games and violent
criminal offenses from the National Incident Based Reporting System (NIBRS) for each week of 2005 to 2008. We instrument for game sales with game characteristics, game quality and time on the market, and estimate that, while a one percent increase in
violent games is associated with up to a 0.03% decrease in violent crime, non-violent games appear to have no effect on crime rates.
Scott Cunningham of Baylor University Benjamin Engelstatter of Centre for European Economic Research (ZEW) - Information and Communication Technologies Research Group Michael R. Ward of University of Texas at Arlington - College of Business Administration - Department of Economics
April 7, 2011
|
|
Researchers say the study was the first to show a clear link between a sustained period of playing violent games and subsequent increases in hostile behaviour
|
|
|
|
18th October 2012
|
|
| See article from ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
See also article from
dailymail.co.uk
|
A longitudinal study of the association between violent video game play and aggression among adolescents. Willoughby T, Adachi PJ, Good M. Department of Psychology, Brock University, Ontario, Canada Abstract
In the past 2 decades, correlational and experimental studies have found a positive association between violent video game play and aggression. There is less evidence, however, to support a long-term relation between these behaviors.
This study examined sustained violent video game play and adolescent aggressive behavior across the high school years and directly assessed the socialization (violent video game play predicts aggression over time) versus selection
hypotheses (aggression predicts violent video game play over time). Adolescents were surveyed annually from Grade 9 to Grade 12 about their video game play and aggressive behaviors. Nonviolent video game play, frequency of
overall video game play, and a comprehensive set of potential 3rd variables were included as covariates in each analysis. Sustained violent video game play was significantly related to steeper increases in adolescents' trajectory
of aggressive behavior over time. Moreover, greater violent video game play predicted higher levels of aggression over time, after controlling for previous levels of aggression, supporting the socialization hypothesis. In
contrast, no support was found for the selection hypothesis. Nonviolent video game play also did not predict higher levels of aggressive behavior over time. Our findings, and the fact that many adolescents play video games for several hours every day,
underscore the need for a greater understanding of the long-term relation between violent video games and aggression, as well as the specific game characteristics (e.g., violent content, competition, pace of action) that may be responsible for this
association. The Daily Mail researched a few press release quotes. Lead researcher Professor Teena Willoughby said: The current study is the first to demonstrate a relation between sustained
violent video game play and the progression of aggressive behaviour. It is clear that there is a long-term association between violent video games and aggression. This is an important and concerning finding, particularly in light
of the hours that youth spend playing these games.
Professor Mark Griffiths, director of the International Gaming Research Unit at Nottingham Trent University, said: The study as a whole does
provide one of the strongest pieces of empirical evidence to date that there is a direct relationship between playing violent video games and subsequent aggressive behaviour.'
|
15th April 2012 | | |
Why are video games claimed to cause violent aggression when in fact players need to learn to keep their cool and use calm rational thinking?
| See article from
sciencedaily.com
|
There is a long-lasting and at times intense debate about the possible link between violent computer games and aggressiveness. A group of researchers from the University of Gothenburg, Sweden, are now questioning the entire basis of the discussion.
In a recently published article, they present a new study showing that, more than anything, a good ability to cooperate is a prerequisite for success in the violent gaming environment. A study, authored by Ulrika Bennerstedt, Jonas Ivarsson and
Jonas Linderoth and titled How gamers manage aggression: Situating skills in collaborative computer games , is presented in International Journal of Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning. The Gothenburg-based research group spent
hundreds of hours playing online games and observing other gamers, including on video recordings. They focused on complex games with portrayals of violence and aggressive action where the participants have to fight with and against each other. The
situations gamers encounter in these games call for sophisticated and well-coordinated collaboration. We analysed what characteristics and knowledge the gamers need to have in order to be successful, says Jonas Ivarsson, Docent (Reader) at the
Department of Education, Communication and Learning. It turns out that a successful gamer is strategic and technically knowledgeable, and has good timing. Inconsiderate gamers, as well as those who act aggressively or emotionally, generally do not
do well. In a nutshell, we're questioning the whole gaming and violence debate, since it's not based on a real problem but rather on some hypothetical reasoning, says Ivarsson.
|
5th March 2012 | | |
Research finds that computer game developers depict religion as violent and problematic
| See article from christianpost.com
|
Recent video games have begun depicting religion as a violent, problematic force, according to research from a new University of Missouri study. Greg Perreault, a doctoral student at University of Missouri's School of Journalism, studied five
extremely popular games from the last few years that incorporate religion heavily into their storylines: Mass Effect 2, Final Fantasy XIII, Assassin's Creed, Castlevania: Lords of Shadow , and The Elder Scrolls IV: Oblivion. In each case, Perreault found that religion became equated with violence within the video games' narratives. Perreault said in a press release:
In most of these games there was a heavy emphasis on a 'Knights Templar' and crusader motifs. Not only was the violent side of religion emphasized, but in each of these games religion created a problem that the main
character must overcome, whether it is a direct confrontation with religious zealots or being haunted by religious guilt.
Just because religion was associated with violence, however, does not mean it was always depicted as evil. For
example, Perreault noted that in Mass Effect 2 , the character of Thane is an extremely spiritual assassin who assists the player. Of those five games, Mass Effect 2, Final Fantasy XIII, and The Elder Scrolls IV: Oblivion all deal
with religions created specifically for the game. The remaining two titles, Assassin's Creed and Castlevania: Lords of Shadow , both center around Catholicism. Still, Perreault emphasized that he did not believe game
developers were attacking religion with these themes: It doesn't appear that game developers are trying to purposefully bash organized religion in these games. I believe they are only using religion to create
stimulating plot points in their story lines. If you look at video games across the board, most of them involve violence in some fashion because violence is conflict and conflict is exciting. Religion appears to get tied in with violence because that
makes for a compelling narrative.
While Perreault's study of just five games is far from an exhaustive survey of all of modern video games, he does believe game writers should be aware of how they use religion in their plots.
|
7th December 2011 | | |
The Swedish Media Council finds no evidence that computer games cause aggressive behaviour
| See article from gamepolitics.com
|
A new report from the Swedish Media Council comes to the conclusion that there's no conclusive evidence that there is no evidence that violent computer games cause aggressive behavior . The Media Council is a Swedish government agency in
charge of film and media classification and whose mission statement is to reduce the risk of harmful media influences among minors and to empower minors as conscious media users. The findings are based on a review of more than 100 articles
about violent games and aggression which have been published in international scientific journals since 2000. The review found that there is a clear and statistically significant link between violent games and aggressive behavior. But the review also
found that many of those same studies use different methods to measure aggression, and few produced a clear connection to violent behavior. Many of those same studies suffered from serious methodological deficiencies and didn't provide sufficient
evidence to establish a causal relationship. The studies that did attempt to examine other causes of aggression found that factors such as poor physical health or family problems were factors that lead to violent behavior and a propensity to play
violent games. If research can't provide any simple answers about how games make children aggressive, perhaps we adults should stop judging the games children play based on whether they are violent or not, Media Council researcher Ulf
Dalquist said in a statement. |
30th November 2011 | | |
Anti game nutters support research that finds diminished brain activity in the anterior cingulate cortex of gamers
| See press release
from prnewswire.com
|
A functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) analysis of long-term effects of violent video game play on the brain has found changes in brain regions associated with cognitive function and emotional control in young adult men after one week of game
play. The results of the study were presented at the annual meeting of the Radiological Society of North America (RSNA). The controversy over whether or not violent video games are potentially harmful to users has raged for many
years, making it as far as the Supreme Court in 2010. But there has been little scientific evidence demonstrating that the games have a prolonged negative neurological effect. For the first time, we have found that a sample of
randomly assigned young adults showed less activation in certain frontal brain regions following a week of playing violent video games at home, said Yang Wang, M.D., assistant research professor in the Department of Radiology and Imaging Sciences at
Indiana University School of Medicine in Indianapolis. These brain regions are important for controlling emotion and aggressive behavior. For the study, 22 healthy adult males, age 18 to 29, with low past exposure to
violent video games were randomly assigned to two groups of 11. Members of the first group were instructed to play a shooting video game for 10 hours at home for one week and refrain from playing the following week. The second group did not play a
violent video game at all during the two-week period. Each of the 22 men underwent fMRI at the beginning of the study, with follow-up exams at one and two weeks. The results showed that after one week of violent game play, the
video game group members showed less activation in the left inferior frontal lobe during the emotional task and less activation in the anterior cingulate cortex during the counting task, compared to their baseline results and the results of the control
group after one week. After the second week without game play, the changes to the executive regions of the brain were diminished. These findings indicate that violent video game play has a long-term effect on brain functioning,
Dr. Wang said. Coauthors are Tom Hummer, Ph.D., William Kronenberger, Ph.D., Kristine Mosier, D.M.D., Ph.D., and Vincent P. Mathews, M.D. This research is supported by the Center for Successful Parenting, Indiana. Game
Politics points out that the Center for Successful Parenting, Indiana is in fact a nutter group with a website that is designed for parents to learn about the negative side effects of violent video.
|
30th October 2011 | |
| Preference for Violent Electronic Games and Aggressive Behavior among Children: The Beginning of the Downward Spiral?
| Based on article from
citeulike.org by: Maria von Salisch, Jens Vogelgesang, Astrid Kristen, Caroline Oppl
|
Abstract A one-year longitudinal study with 324 German third and fourth graders was conducted in order to find out whether a preference for violent electronic games socializes children to become more aggressive or whether
aggressive individuals tend to select this type of game. Cross-lagged panel analyses suggest that children who were rated as openly aggressive at Time 1 intensified their preference for violent electronic games over
time. We determined that it could be ruled out that this selection effect was due to a number of underlying variables ranging from ecological variables (neighborhood) to family variables (migration status, older brother) and child variables (gender,
self-esteem, level of achievement). Press Based on article
from monstersandcritics.com The research suggested there was a risk that this preference for violent video games would become entrenched in these children.
However, the researchers found no evidence in the group they studied that violent computer and video games led to increased aggression in real life. This is the good news from our study into the educational effects of media, said Jens
Vogelgesang of Hohenheim University: But it should be noted that this applies expressly only to the group of 8-to-12 year olds that we looked into in a study on the effects. One of the researchers seems disappointed that results don't
support the concept that games cause violence. The research team leader Maria von Salisch throws in a totally out of context comment: In the case of older children, the negative effects from violent games on their
behaviour has already been documented and this remains a cause for concern. We are unable to rule out the possibility that an entrenched preference for violent computer and video games might over the course of a
game-playing career lead to greater readiness to commit acts of violence.
What sort of researchers title their supposed scientific study with nutter phrases like 'downward spiral' anyway?
|
21st October 2011 | | |
Mormon University finds that strong language on TV is the first step on the slippery slope to aggressive behaviour
| See
article from dailymail.co.uk
|
Being exposed to strong language on TV as well as playing video games are linked to aggression in teenagers, a university report shows. A US study in the medical journal Pediatrics appears to be the first to examine the impact of strong
language. To explore the issue, scholars at the mormon Brigham Young University in Utah gathered information from 223 middle school students. Family life professor at the university, Sarah Coyne, explained that the findings revealed that
exposure to bad language is associated with acceptance and use of similar language, which in turn influences both physical and verbal aggression. Professor Coyne said: On the whole, it's a moderate effect.
We even ran the statistical model the opposite way to test if the violent kids used more profanity and then sought it out in the media, but the first path we took was a much better statistical fit even when we tried other
explanations. Profanity is kind of like a stepping stone. You don't go to a movie, hear a bad word, and then go and shoot somebody. But when youth both hear and then try profanity out for themselves it can start a downward slide
toward more aggressive behavior.
|
18th October 2011 | |
| Brain activity patterns found to differ between gamers and non-gamers
| Based on press release from
www3.uni-bonn.de
|
Researchers from the University of Bonn have found brain activity patterns in heavy gamers that differed from those of non-gamers. The study's results have just been published in the scientific journal Biological Psychology. Psychologists,
epileptologists and neurologists from the University of Bonn studied the effect of shoot em up game images and other emotionally charged photos on the brain activity of heavy gamers. Compared to people who abstain from first-person shooters, they show
clear differences in how emotions are controlled, reported lead author Dr. Christian Montag from the Institute of Psychology at the University of Bonn. 21 subjects ranging in age from 20 to 30 years played first-person shooters for about 15
hours per week on average. During this study, they were shown a standardized catalog of photos that reliably trigger emotions in human brains, using video glasses. At the same time, the researchers recorded the responses in their brains using one of the
brain scanners at the Life & Brain Center of the University of Bonn. The images included photos as they are used in the violent games, but also shots of accident and disaster victims. This mix of images allowed us to transport the subjects both to
the fictitious first-person shooter world they are familiar with and to also trigger emotions via real images, explained Dr. Montag. This catalog of photos was also shown to a control group of 19 persons who had no experience with violent video
games. When the subjects regarded the real, negative pictures, there was greatly increased activity in their amygdalas. This region of the brain is strongly involved in processing negative emotions. Surprisingly, the amygdalas in the subjects
as well as in the control group were similarly stimulated, reported Montag: This shows that both groups responded to the photos with similarly strong emotions. But the left medial frontal lobes were clearly less activated in the users
of violent games than in the control subjects. This is the brain structure humans use to control their fear or aggression. First-person shooters do not respond as strongly to the real, negative image material because they are used to it from their
daily computer activities, Montag concluded: One might also say that they are more desensitized than the control group. On the other hand, while processing the computer game images, the first-person shooters showed higher activity in
brain regions associated with memory recall and working memory than the control group members. This indicates that the gamers put themselves into the video game due to the computer game images and were looking for a potential strategy to find a
solution for the game status shown, said Dr. Montag. One question raised while interpreting the results is whether the users showed altered brain activity due to the games, or whether they were more tolerant of violence from the start and as a
consequence, preferred first-person shooter games. The researchers from the University of Bonn were able to suggest an answer to this question based on the fact that they took into account various personality traits such as fearfulness, aggressiveness,
callousness or emotional stability. There were no differences between the subjects and the control group in this area, reported Dr. Montag: This is an indication that the violent games are the cause of the difference in information processing
in the brain.
|
23rd September 2011 | | |
NHS slap down Daily Mail hype about a new direction of research into the psychology of games playing
| Thanks to MiichaelG and Wynter
| In the wake of Grand
Theft Auto being mentioned in a murder trial, the Daily Mail seized upon a piece of research about computer gaming. The Daily Mail reported: How video games blur real life boundaries and prompt thoughts of
violent solutions to players' problems See
article from
dailymail.co.uk . By Jenny Hope
Some video game players are transferring their screen experiences into the real world - prompting thoughts of violent solutions to their problems, say researchers. Fans of computers can become so immersed in their virtual
environment they do things in the real world as if they were still playing. The findings come after sailor Ryan Donovan was sentenced to 25 years in jail for shooting dead an officer on a nuclear sub to copy the violent video game
Grand Theft Auto. Researchers at Nottingham Trent University and Stockholm University have for the first time identified evidence of Game Transfer Phenomena (GTP), which results in some gamers integrating video experiences into
their real lives. The study to be published in the next issue of the International Journal of Cyber Behaviour, Psychology and Learning. The study involved 42 in-depth interviews with participants aged between 15 and 21 years old,
all of whom were frequent video game players and had been recruited from gaming forums. They thought in the same way as when they were gaming, with half of participants often looking to use something from a video game to resolve a
real-life issue. In some cases these thoughts were accompanied by reflexes, such as reaching to click a button on the controller when it wasn't in their hands, while on other occasions gamers visualised their thoughts in the form
of game menus. ... Violent solutions to real life conflicts appeared to be used by few of the players, at least in their imaginations says the study. One 15-year-old gamer
said: There (in the video game) you can get guns. This I want to do in real life, to get some guns, shoot down people. This I want to do sometimes with irritating people. The study concluded: The close resemblance to
real life scenarios in video games may have opened a 'Pandora's Box for some players. ...Read the full
article
The UK's national health
service has responded to this Daily Mail write up and added a much needed bit of perspective: Video games blur reality , claims newspaper From
nhs.uk
The Daily Mail has today reported that video games blur real life boundaries and prompt thoughts of violent solutions to players' problems . This headline is based on a small study exploring whether frequent video game
players integrated elements of video game playing into their real lives - a theoretical process the researchers called game transfer phenomena (GTP). The study showed that most gamers experienced GTP, including experiencing brief involuntary impulses to
perform actions as they would when playing a game. For example, they might try to click a button on their controller while it was not in their hand. It is important to note that not all the players were affected by the games and
the degree that people were affected varied significantly from person to person. Additionally, it is not clear from this study whether GTP was related to the game played or whether it related to the specific characteristics of individual game players.
Many of the actions reported by participants were also unusual or novel, and do not provide evidence that games affect perception of behaviour. For example, one participant said that they like to pack their suitcase neatly like Tetris blocks.
Further studies will be needed to investigate whether GTP is a real, significant phenomenon and the potential link between GTP and a player's individual characteristics. ... The Daily
Mail's report covering this study tended to focus on the violent and negative aspects of game transfer phenomena (GTP) highlighted in the study. The Daily Mail presents GTP as a proven phenomenon with definite results, but the results of this
interview-based study are debatable and GTP is still only a theory. News coverage also linked the study results to a recent murder trial where video games were reportedly implicated. This angle seemed to be a confused addition to
news coverage of the research, as it could suggest to readers that games were found to be the primary cause of the incident, or that they could cause ordinary people to consider murder.
|
3rd September 2011 | | |
Competitive games more likely to cause aggression than violent games
| See article from gamepolitics.com See research report
The Effect of Video Game Competition and Violence on Aggressive Behavior: Which Characteristic Has the Greatest Influence? [pdf]
|
Research from Brock University in Canada seems to indicate that playing highly competitive video games may lead to aggressive behavior faster than playing games with more violent content. Competitiveness, says a new study published by the American
Psychological Association, may be the main video game characteristic that influences or causes aggression. In a series of experiments lead by Paul J.C. Adachi, M.A., a PhD candidate at Brock University in Canada, video games were matched on
competitiveness, difficulty, and pace of action. Researchers found that video game violence did not elevate aggressive behavior on its own. The more competitive games produced greater levels of aggressive behavior than less competitive
games, no matter how much violent content was found in the games. In one of the experiments, 42 college students played one of two video games, Conan or Fuel , for 12 minutes. Both games were even when it came to competitiveness,
difficulty and pace of action, but differed in levels of violence. After participants finished playing the game, they were then told they were going to take part in a separate food tasting study. Participants had to make up a cup of hot sauce for a taster
who they were told did not particularly like hot or spicy food. The participants could choose from one of four different hot sauces (from least hot to most hot) for the taster to drink. The authors found that there was no significant difference in
the intensity and amount of the hot sauces prepared by the participants who played Conan and those who played Fuel. The authors concluded that video game violence alone was not sufficient to elevate aggressive behavior. In the second
experiment, 60 college students played one of four video games: Mortal Kombat versus DC Universe , Left 4 Dead 2 , Marble Blast Ultra , and Fuel . Afterward, the students completed the same hot sauce test from the first study.
Electrocardiograms measured the participants' heart rates before and during video game play. On average, students who played the highly competitive games - Fuel and Mortal Kombat versus DC Universe - concocted what researchers called significantly more of a hotter sauce
than participants who played Marble Blast Ultra and Left 4 Dead 2. They also had significantly higher heart rates. Adachi concluded: These findings suggest that the level of competitiveness in video
games is an important factor in the relation between video games and aggressive behavior, with highly competitive games leading to greater elevations in aggression than less competitive games.
|
27th June 2011 | | |
Violent video games lead to decreases in violent crime
| ie Violent video games may contribute to violent behaviour...BUT...they also keep gamers off the streets...Overall.. this leads to reduction in violent crime
Based on article from papers.ssrn.com
|
Psychological studies invariably find a positive relationship between violent video game play and aggression. However, these studies cannot account for either aggressive effects of alternative activities video game playing substitutes for or the possible
selection of relatively violent people into playing violent video games. That is, they lack external validity. We investigate the relationship between the prevalence of violent video games and violent crimes. Our results are consistent with
two opposing effects. First, they support the behavioral effects as in the psychological studies. Second, they suggest a larger voluntary incapacitation effect in which playing either violent or non-violent games decrease crimes. Overall, violent
video games lead to decreases in violent crime.
|
9th May 2011 | |
| British games players get more worked up about football than shoot 'em ups
| See article from gamepolitics.com
|
According to a group of researchers in England, games with goals such as football are more likely to make participants aggressive than anything encountered in Grand Theft Auto or Call Of Duty . According to research conducted by Dr.
Simon Goodson and Sarah Pearson of Huddersfield university, games with goals cause more of an aggressive reaction in participants than killing an animated character because sports is closer to real life. Researchers measured the heart rates,
respiration and brain activity of 40 male and female participants randomly selected to play violent Xbox 360 game or a football game. They found that when players killed someone in a game it caused little brain activity. But when participant's conceded a
goal or foul in the sports game it caused a higher level of brain activity. Dr. Goodson added that participants generally reacted with more agitation during the football game and that maybe violent games have been misrepresented as the worst thing
a gamer can play. Dr. Goodson is presenting his research this week at the British Psychological Society's annual conference in Glasgow, Scotland. |
19th February 2011 | | | Violent video games don't desensitise players to violence
| From gamepolitics.com
|
According to researchers at Ryerson University (Toronto, Canada), violent video games do not desensitize players to violent imagery. The study was lead by Holly Bowen and co-authored by psychology professor Julia Spaniol. Researchers examined
the impact of chronic exposure to violent video games on emotional memory and responses to negative stimuli. Emotional long-term memory helps us avoid negative situations, Bowen said. This has significant implications for public health.
For example, if you remember the negative experience of being involved in a bar fight, you will avoid future situations that may lead to an altercation. Participants were shown 150 images representing three different stimuli: negative,
positive and neutral scenes. One hour later, the students viewed those same images again (along with a new set of 150 distractor images) in random order. As each image was displayed, participants had to respond whether or not they had seen it
before. Finally, at the end of the experiment, the students completed a self-assessment test regarding their state of emotional arousal. The researchers believed going into the study that game players would prove to be less sensitive to the
negative images and therefore show reduced memory for these materials. The results showed no difference in the memory of video game players and non-players. Exposure to video games were not associated with differences in self-reported arousal to
emotional stimuli. The findings indicate that long-term emotional memory is not affected by chronic exposure violent video games, said Bowen. Researchers caution that further study is needed to see if these results apply to all age groups
and not just young adults.
|
19th December 2010 | |
| Researcher finds that depression is a better predictor of aggression than violent games playing
| Based on article from gamepolitics.com
|
New research by Dr. Christopher Ferguson from Texas A&M International University finds that depression in young people has more of a correlation to aggressive and violent behavior than gaming does - at least among Hispanics. Ferguson recruited
302 (mostly) Hispanic youths between the ages of 10 - 14 years-old, from a small city on the border of Mexico. The population of this unnamed city was primarily of Hispanic dissent. Participants were interviewed at the start of the study and at the end
of the study 12 months later. He then looked at how much exposure the subjects had to violence in video games, television, and negative events in their lives. Negative events included neighborhood problems, bad relationships with adults,
antisocial behavior, family attachment, delinquent peers, family interaction and communication, exposure to domestic violence, depressive symptoms, serious aggression, bullying, and delinquent behavior. One year later, 7% reported being involved
in at least one criminally violent act during the previous 12 months, with the common crime being physical assaults on other students or the use of force to take something away from someone else. 19% reported engaging in at least one nonviolent
crime during the same period, such as shoplifting or theft on school property. Ferguson found that symptoms of depressions were a strong predictor for youth aggression and rule breaking. Depression was especially influential in those who
were identified as having preexisting antisocial personality traits. The research did not find that exposure to violence from video games or television at the start of the study was a good predictor of aggressive behavior in young people. Dr.
Furgeson's research will appear in the Journal of Youth and Adolescence. |
20th October 2010 | | |
Researcher finds that viewers of violence get bored and so less responsive
| Based on article from
bbc.co.uk
|
Repeated viewing of violent scenes in films, television or video games could make teenagers behave more aggressively, US research claims. The National Institutes of Health study of 22 boys aged 14 to 17 found that showing dozens of violent clips
appeared to blunt brain responses. Dr Jordan Grafman said it might make aggression feel more acceptable . The US study, published in the journal Social Cognitive & Affective Neuroscience , involved 60 violent scenes from videos
being collated, mostly involving street brawling and fist fights. The violence was ranked low , mild or moderate , and there were no extreme scenes. The response of the boys as they watched the clips was measured in a
number of ways. They were asked to rate whether they thought each clip was more or less aggressive than the one which preceded it, and were brain scanned using functional magnetic resonance imaging, which shows in real time which areas of the brain are
active.In addition, electrodes attached to the fingers detected increasing sweat - a sign of an emotional response. The longer the boys watched videos, particularly the mild or moderate ones, the less they responded to the violence within them.
In particular, an area of the brain known as the lateral orbitofrontal cortex, thought to be involved in emotional processing, showed less activity to each clip as time went on. Dr Grafman said: Exposure to the most violent videos
inhibits emotional reactions to similar aggressive videos over time and implies that normal adolescents will feel fewer emotions over time as they are exposed to similar videos. The implications of this include the idea that continued exposure to violent
videos will make an adolescent less sensitive to violence, more accepting of violence, and more likely to commit aggressive acts since the emotional component associated with aggression is reduced and normally acts as a brake on aggressive behaviour.
However, another academic said it was almost impossible to explain violence in these terms. Professor David Buckingham, the director of the Centre for the Study of Children, Youth and Media, said that violence was a social problem with many
contributing factors, not simply a matter of looking at how the brain worked: The suggestion is that, over a period of time, people can develop a kind of tolerance to these images - but another word for that is just boredom.
|
15th September 2010 | |
| Decisions that is
| Based on article
from independent.co.uk
|
A new study into the effects of computer games has revealed that fast-paced action games turn us into faster and better decision-makers. Scientists at the University of Rochester in New York conducted a series of tests to gauge whether regular
bouts of high-speed gaming could help to improve our cognitive abilities. The researchers tested dozens of 18- to 25-year-olds who were not ordinarily video-gamers, splitting them into two groups. The first group were told to play
adrenalin-packed action games such as Call of Duty 2 and Unreal Tournament , in which participants dash around online arenas shooting each other. The second group were given The Sims 2 , a more sedate, strategy-based game that mimics
the pace of everyday life. After 50 hours of playing, both groups were given a series of tests to see whether they could make quicker decisions. Scientists discovered that those who had trained on the action games made decisions 25% faster than
their counterparts. They also answered just as many questions correctly as their strategy game-playing peers. It's not the case that the action game players are trigger-happy and less accurate – they are just as accurate and also faster, said Daphne Bavelier, a cognitive scientist at Rochester who has been testing how computer games affect the brain and eyes for much of the past 10 years.
Action game players make more correct decisions per unit time. If you are a surgeon or you are in the middle of a battlefield, that can make all the difference. This benefit, researchers suggest in a forthcoming edition of Current
Biology , has repercussions in the real world, such as improving our ability to multitask, drive, read small print and keep track of friends in a crowd. Decisions are never black and white, she said. The brain is always computing
probabilities. As you drive, for instance, you may see a movement on your right, estimate whether you are on a collision course, and based on that probability make a binary decision: brake or don't brake. Action-filled computer games – which
force the brain to make a whole series of fast-paced decisions in a split second – appear to improve our ability to make those decisions quickly. Shooting off a few rounds of nonsense sound bites Based
on
article from news.sky.com
But Vivienne Pattison, director of Mediawatch-UK, told Sky News the study could send out the wrong message about shoot 'em ups: I dont dispute the findings, it is going to improve you reactions if you click something enough,
of course it is, but I'm not just talking about mental reactions - these games dehumanise violence. There have been other studies that link them to violence and you could get the same reactions from a driving game.
|
14th July 2010 | |
| Violent Video Games Help Relieve Stress, Depression
| Based on article from
tamiu.edu
|
Young adults—male and female—who play violent video games long-term, handle stress better than non-playing adults and become less depressed and less hostile following a stressful task, according to a study by Texas A&M International University
associate professor, Dr. Christopher J. Ferguson. The article on the research appears in the European Psychologist 2010. In this study, 103 young adults were given a frustration task and then randomized to play
no game, a non-violent game, a violent game with good versus evil theme, or a violent game in which they played 'the bad guy.' The results suggest that violent games reduce depression and hostile feelings in players through mood management, Dr.
Ferguson explained. Whether violent video games cause aggression or violent crime has been a source of contention in public and academic circles. The results do not support a link between violent video games and aggressive behavior. Ferguson said that the results of this study may help provide others with ways to come up with a mood-management activity that provides individuals with ways to tolerate or reduce stress:
It probably won't come to a surprise to gamers that playing games may reduce stress, although others have been skeptical of this idea. This is the first study that explores this idea, however. It does seem that playing violent games
may help reduce stress and make people less depressed and hostile.
|
15th June 2010 | |
| Vulnerability to Violent Video Games
| Based on article
from newswise.com See also Vulnerability to Violent Video Games [pdf]
from apa.org
|
In a special issue of the journal Review of General Psychology, published in June by the American Psychological Association, researchers looked at several studies that examined the potential uses of video games as a way to improve visual/spatial skills,
as a health aid to help manage diabetes or pain and as a tool to complement psychotherapy. One study examined the negative effects of violent video games on some people. Much of the attention to video game research has been negative, focusing
on potential harm related to addiction, aggression and lowered school performance, said Christopher J. Ferguson, PhD, of Texas A&M International University and guest editor of the issue. Recent research has shown that as video games have
become more popular, children in the United States and Europe are having fewer behavior problems, are less violent and score better on standardized tests. Violent video games have not created the generation of problem youth so often feared. In
contrast, one study in the special issue shows that video game violence can increase aggression in some individuals, depending on their personalities. In his research, Patrick Markey, PhD, determined that a certain combination of personality
traits can help predict which young people will be more adversely affected by violent video games. Previous research has shown us that personality traits like psychoticism and aggressiveness intensify the negative effects of violent video games and we
wanted to find out why, said Markey. Markey used the most popular psychological model of personality traits, called the Five-Factor Model, to examine these effects. The model scientifically classifies five personality traits: neuroticism,
extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness and conscientiousness. Analysis of the model showed a perfect storm of traits for children who are most likely to become hostile after playing violent video games, according to Markey.
Those traits are: high neuroticism (e.g., easily upset, angry, depressed, emotional, etc.), low agreeableness (e.g., little concern for others, indifferent to others feelings, cold, etc.) and low conscientiousness (e.g., break rules, don't keep promises,
act without thinking, etc.). Markey then created his own model, focusing on these three traits, and used it to help predict the effects of violent video games in a sample of 118 teenagers. Each participant played a violent or a non-violent video
game and had his or her hostility levels assessed. The teenagers who were highly neurotic, less agreeable and less conscientious tended to be most adversely affected by violent video games, whereas participants who did not possess these personality
characteristics were either unaffected or only slightly negatively affected by violent video games. These results suggest that it is the simultaneous combination of these personality traits which yield a more powerful predictor of violent video
games, said Markey. Those who are negatively affected have pre-existing dispositions, which make them susceptible to such violent media. Violent video games are like peanut butter, said Ferguson. They are harmless for the vast
majority of kids but are harmful to a small minority with pre-existing personality or mental health problems.
|
5th June 2010 | | |
Researcher finds the 2 hours of violent games playing can influence the judgments of potential jurors
| Based on article from gamepolitics.com
|
Inspired by a plotline from the 2003 movie Runaway Jury , University of Southern California researcher Kwan Min Lee, and associates, conducted research under the title Will the Experience of Playing a Violent Role in a Videogame Influence
People's Judgments of Violent Crimes? The inspiring scene from Runaway Jury involved the lawyer for a defendant accused of committing a violent shooting tried to have a hardcore video game player selected as a member of the jury.
The lawyer carried the belief that a hardcore gamer would judge the shooter less negatively because of similar, though virtual, experiences. 52 undergrads were involved in the study—who had never played the game used in the experiment ( True Crime
)—and were randomly assigned to either a game-playing group or a control group. The game-playing group was tasked with playing True Crime for 2 hours. Both groups were then asked to read two real-life crime cases committed by police officers and
two by generic criminals, and then to answer a series of questions in which they judged the crime and the criminal. It was reported that, people who played the violent game had more favorable judgment of the crimes and perpetrators than
people who did not play the violent game in terms of their negative judgment of the perpetrators. The researchers concluded that that even a few hours of game playing as a particular game character can significantly influence one's attitude
towards real-life criminal behaviors conducted by an individual similar to the game character. It was also theorized that perhaps the jury selection process should take into account both real and virtual experiences as it was shown that
attitudes towards real criminals can be influenced while taking on a similar role in a videogame.
|
29th April 2010 | | |
Shoot 'em up games players learn more flexibility for multi tasking
| From gamepolitics.com
|
Games may not bad for you after all, according to a joint study conducted by the Department of Psychology at the University of Amsterdam, and Leiden University in the Netherlands. More precisely, first-person shooters are better at training you
for the hustle and bustle of everyday life, and less effective at making you a killing machine. DOOM'd to switch: superior cognitive flexibility in players of first person shooter games, a research paper published on Frontiers in Cognition
focuses on the benefits of playing first-person shooters. Thirty-four adults (17 video game players and 17 non-gamers) participated in the study, with video game players defined as those who have played video games at least four times a week
for a minimum period of 6 months. All of the video game players had some experience with games like Call of Duty: Modern Warfare, Unreal Tournament, Battlefield , and Grand Theft Auto IV . Non-gamers were described as having little
to no video game experience. All of the participants were recruited among student populations and through advertisements on internet forums catering to video game players. While details of the study are pretty complex, the short story
is that the video game players were able to rapidly react to fast moving visual and auditory stimuli, and to switch back and forth between different subtasks faster than the non-video game playing group. The study also conducted test to make sure that
these results weren't based on age or I.Q. After eliminating those possibilities, researchers found that it had more to do with first-person shooter games than any other possible factors.
|
1st January 2010 | | |
Research points out benefits of video games
| Based on article from gamepolitics.com
|
A new study conducted by a Wheaton College professor has concluded that people that play action and puzzle games are better able to think through complex problems. Rolf Nelson, a professor of psychology, conducted the study and published his
findings in the November edition of the journal Perception. In the study, he had 20 students try to solve a spatial relation problem. The students were then given a puzzle game or action game to play. Once done with the game, the students were
given the chance to finish the spatial relation problem again. Results showed that puzzle players finished the task slower, but with more accuracy, while action players finished the task quicker but less accurately. Both finished quicker than if
they had not played a game at all. From the study abstract: To understand the way in which video-game play affects subsequent perception and cognitive strategy, two experiments were performed in which
participants played either a fast-action game or a puzzle-solving game. Before and after video-game play, participants performed a task in which both speed and accuracy were emphasized. In experiment 1 participants engaged in a location task in which
they clicked a mouse on the spot where a target had appeared, and in experiment 2 they were asked to judge which of four shapes was most similar to a target shape. In both experiments, participants were much faster but less accurate after playing the
action game, while they were slower but more accurate after playing the puzzle game. Results are discussed in terms of a taxonomy of video games by their cognitive and perceptual demands.
|
26th December 2009 | |
| Education minister points out benefits of video games
| From thescotsman.scotsman.com
|
Education minister Mike Russell said the technology, often seen as a distraction, could motivate young people to develop skills. Russell said the Nintendo DS console, for example, could lead to surprising educational benefits for young people,
particularly its top-selling brain-training games. He said: We need to embrace new technologies and tap into all the resources available to us to ensure that our young people develop successfully in a modern society within which
computers are so important. Educational computer games can be a great way of motivating young people to learn in a way that is relevant and enjoyable for them. Computer games are often perceived as solely a distraction to learning. But
alongside traditional learning aids, they can help make learning more engaging. Parents and teachers across the country are starting to see the benefits they can have. Dr David Miller of the University of Dundee has conducted studies
into the effects of brain-training games on improving learning in partnership with government education body Learning and Teaching Scotland (LTS). Miller said it was clear many games had tremendous learning potential. The research focusing on a
brain-training game showed it could improve pupil's basic computational skills. Children who used the game for 20 minutes each morning over a ten-week period demonstrated statistically significant gains in accuracy and speed of processing. He
said: Research is starting to point towards computer games providing some real and tangible benefits to young people. These can include faster processing of information, enhanced selection of relevant material and higher levels of engagement. Computer
games are part of our culture and while we may have concerns about aspects of some popular games, many have huge potential for supporting learning.
|
23rd December 2009 | | |
Researchers find the good side to playing video games
| From telegraph.co.uk
|
University of Rochester researchers found that children who played video games were quick thinkers and had good hand-eye co-ordination. Contrary to the widely held belief that video games are contributing to Britain's obesity crisis, a team of
American researchers has found that there are benefits to sitting in front of a screen for hours on end. Dr Matthew Dye said there were good points about playing video games even though many critics claim they distract children from more healthy
outdoor pursuits: Avid players got faster not only on their game of choice, but on a variety of unrelated laboratory tests of reaction time . Sceptics agree that gamers are fast but that they become less accurate as their speed of play
increases. Gamers don't lose accuracy in the game or in lab tests as they get faster - this is a result of the gamer's improved visual cognition. Dr Dye said video game fans did well on hand-eye co-ordination tests and comprehension tests: Playing video games enhances performance on mental rotation skills, visual and spatial memory, and tasks requiring divided attention.
Dr Dye said that parents and grandparents should play their children's computer games to improve their own memory and brain function: Training with video games may serve to reduce gender differences in visual and spatial processing, and
thwart some of the cognitive declines that come with ageing .
|
17th October 2009 | | |
Researchers link frequent game playing with Attention Deficit Disorder
| Based on article from gamepolitics.com
|
Results of a study performed by researchers at Iowa State University have led them to believe that there is a relation between frequent videogame playing and Attention Deficit Disorder (ADD). Video Games and Cognitive Control was
designed to quantify the effects of playing videogames on two types of cognitive activity—proactive and reactive. Proactive attention is described as a gearing up mechanism, or where a player can anticipate what is coming next, versus reactive
attention, which is more of a knee-jerk response (a monster jumping out). A visual task was used to test both attention types with brain waves and responses measured in both frequent videogame players and occasional players. Both groups were
charged with identifying the color of a word when the color and word matched, such as 'RED' presented in red, or did not match, such as 'RED' presented in blue or green. This is also referred to as the Stroop task. While reactive control
was similar in both groups, frequent gamers (participants in this study who play four or more hours a day) had a propensity for exhibiting significantly diminished proactive attention. From a press release: While admitting that the study
did have a few limitations, the researchers hoped that our results may serve to constrain the claims of some scholars, game manufacturers, and journalists who have suggested that playing action video games 'improves attention.' The study is
being published in the October 2009 issue of Psychophysiology.
|
1st October 2009 | |
| Researchers find that any video game aggression is targeted at strangers rather than friends
| Based on article from gamepolitics.com See also
abstract: Hormonal responses differ when playing violent video games against an ingroup and outgroup
|
Did you know that playing violent multiplayer games will make you more aggressive against strangers than friends? That's the conclusion of a new study published in the latest issue of the Evolution and Human Behavior science journal. The study, conducted by psychologists from the University of Missouri, observed 42 young men divided into 14 teams of three. The players played Unreal Tournament 2004 within their team and against other teams. When playing against teammates, the mode was Deathmatch. When playing against other teams, the mode was Onslaught. Before and after each match, the testosterone and cortisol levels of each player was tested.
According to the study's abstract from the journal's web site: For 14 teams of three young men, salivary testosterone and cortisol were assessed twice before and twice after competing in within-group and
between-group video games that simulated violent male-male competition. Men who contributed the most to their teams' between-group victory showed testosterone increases immediately after the competition, but only if this competition was played before the
within-group tournament. High-scoring men on losing teams did not show this immediate effect, but they did show a delayed increase in testosterone. In contrast, high-ranking men tended to have lower testosterone and higher cortisol during within-group
tournaments. The results are consistent with the hypothesis that men's competitive testosterone response varies across ingroup and outgroup competitions and is muted during the former. The testosterone response during the between-group competition also
suggests that violent multiplayer video games may be appealing to young men because they simulate male-male coalitional competition.
|
4th September 2009 | |
| But thick brains may be good brains
| Based on article from gamepolitics.com See also
short report from biomedcentral.com
|
The Researchers at the Mind Research Network found that playing Tetris for three months had increased efficiency and beefed up the amount of gray matter in the brains of a group of adolescent girls. The results, which will be published in BMC
Research Notes later this week, showed that focusing on a challenging visuospatial task such as a video game could not just increase brain activity but alter the structure of the brain as well by thickening the cortexes.
So, what benefit
does a fatter, more efficient brain offer? According to one of the study's authors, Dr. Richard Haier, it may be a way to help combat the mental decline that occurs with age.
|
18th June 2009 | | |
Research claims violent videogames increases hurtful behaviour
| Based on article from gamepolitics.com |
A report published in the current issue of Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin maintains that playing pro-social games increases helping behavior by participants while playing violent games increases hurtful behavior.
GamePolitics has
previously reported on the research, which combines the results from three separate studies conducted in the U.S., Japan and Singapore. But a press release issued today by the University of Michigan offers new insight about the methodologies used by the
researchers involved. UM's Brad Bushman said:
These studies show the same kind of impact on three different age groups from three very different cultures. In addition, the studies use different analytic
approaches---correlational, longitudinal and experimental. The resulting triangulation of evidence provides the strongest possible proof that the findings are both valid and generalizable...
[The research] suggests
there is an upward spiral of prosocial gaming and helpful behavior, in contrast to the downward spiral that occurs with violent video gaming and aggressive behavior...
Taken together, these findings make it clear that playing video games is not
in itself good or bad for children. The type of content in the game has a bigger impact than the overall amount of time spent playing.
Perhaps the most interesting experiment involved 161 U.S. college students. From the press release:
After playing either a prosocial, violent, or neutral game, participants were asked to assign puzzles to a randomly selected partner. They could choose from puzzles that were easy, medium or hard to complete. Their partner could
win $10 if they solved all the puzzles. Those who played a prosocial game were considerably more helpful than others, assigning more easy puzzles to their partners. And those who had played violent games were significantly more likely to assign the
hardest puzzles.
|
18th March 2009 | |
| Multiplying unrelated long odds reveals that violent games provoke aggressive thoughts
| Thanks to Chris My favourite line is: Does that mean playing violent videogames is going to create a school shooter? No, not
if there aren't any other risk factors. But in kids who have a lot of other risk factors, can it contribute to the likelihood of some sort of extreme violent behaviour occurring? Probably, it can. More so than other risk factors? We don't know. There's
no data on it.
Don't let that lack of data get in the way of a good opinion there Professor. Based on article from
uk.games.ign.com
|
In a guest lecture at Macquarie University, Sydney, Professor Anderson, Director of Centre of the Study of Violence at Iowa State University spoke of the risks of violent videogames.
Research was clear by 1975 that media violence caused
aggressive behaviour, Prof. Anderson said: We know that short term exposure to violent media can lead to aggressive behaviour and aggressive thinking within five minutes of watching a violent film or playing a violent game, while long term
exposure can lead to aggression into early adulthood.
To highlight this connection, Prof. Anderson examined the likelihood of violent videogames leading to aggressive behaviour by drawing on well-known examples of cause and effect. Such
examples included the chances of regular consumption of aspirin leading to heart attacks, the chances of asbestos causing cancer, and the chances of condom use reducing the risk of contracting HIV. In all these examples, violent videogames proved to be a
higher risk factor, going as far as being approximately three times more likely to happen than asbestos exposure leading to cancer.
On the scale of youth violence risk factors, violent videogames were more likely to increase aggression than
substance abuse, poverty, and anti-social peers. Violent games are more likely to provoke aggressive thoughts in players.
Anderson was careful to point out that this did not necessarily mean that everyone who played violent videogames would begin
committing violent acts. Rather, violent games made players more prepared to think aggressive thoughts.
He cited another study where college students were asked to play a pro-social, neutral, and violent game, after which each was tested to see
how willing they were to help their peers solve puzzles. The study showed that those who played non-violent, pro-social games were more inclined to be helpful by choosing easier puzzles for their peers to complete, whereas those who had just played
violent games chose difficult puzzles to impede on their peers' ability to complete the challenge.
While Anderson believes that this increase in aggressive behaviour is a cause for concern, he doesn't think that violent games are solely to be
blamed for anti-social behaviour.
Extreme acts of violence always require multiple risk factors being present. You just don't ever have a school shooter, for example, who only has one risk factor. It just doesn't happen. There's usually four,
five, six, seven risk factors, sometimes more. Media violence is one of those risk factors. he said.
Does that mean playing violent videogames is going to create a school shooter? No, not if there aren't any other risk factors. But in kids who
have a lot of other risk factors, can it contribute to the likelihood of some sort of extreme violent behaviour occurring? Probably, it can. More so than other risk factors? We don't know. There's no data on it.
|
3rd March 2009 | | |
Researchers find that high age ratings make games more attractive to youngsters
| Based on article from
chicagotribune.com
|
If you want to make blood-and-gore video games less appealing to minors, toss those restrictive age and violent-content warnings. The lure of something off-limits only increases demand, a new study says.
In the study, researchers tested 310 Dutch
children ranging in age from 7 to 17. Participants read fictitious game descriptions and rated how much or how little they wanted to play each game. In every group, the more objectionable the content, the more kids clamoured for the controller— forbidden fruit,
the researchers called the games. The findings are published in the March issue of Pediatrics.
While research has found that ratings increase the attraction to raunchy TV shows and movies, the hypothesis had never been tested with
video games, reported two of the study's authors, Brad Bushman of the University of Michigan and Elly Konijn of VU University Amsterdam. They suggest that youth should not be allowed to buy their own games, that parents and physicians be aware of
risk factors (such as a drop in grades) and that policy-makers rethink the classifications (such as M, appropriate for those 17 and older), which will only make the games "unspeakably desirable."
|
31st January 2009 | | |
Researcher claims real world responses can be conditioned by computer games
| Based on article
from newscientist.com
|
Volunteers who played a simple cycling game learned to favour one team's jersey and avoid another's. Days later, most subjects subconsciously avoided the same jersey in a real-world test.
As video games become more immersive and realistic, all
involved ought to realise the potential, says Paul Fletcher, a neuroscientist at Cambridge University, UK, who led the study
I don't think this is evidence that video games are bad, says Fletcher, a former gamer: We just need to be
aware that associations formed within the game transfer to the real world – for good or bad. Fletcher and several colleagues recruited 22 volunteer subjects and told them they were testing an experimental sports drink delivery system.
Volunteer played a bicycling game on a laptop with two straws attached to their mouths.
If cyclists from their same team – as indicated by a jersey design – passed by, participants received a slurp of their favourite juice. However,
if a cyclist from the rival team passed the participant, he or she got a swig of salty tea.
Three days later, the same volunteers came back for a follow-up brain scan and a surprise test. Before the scan, Fletcher and his colleagues asked each
subject to sit in a waiting room with two chairs, both with small towels dangling on one arm. One seat corresponded to the insignia of the juice-giving jersey, the other to the symbol for salty tea. Three-quarters of the subjects sat in the chair
that reminded them of juice, though most participants said they did not notice the towel design.
Our research suggests whatever you've learned in the computer game does have an effect on how you behave toward the stimulus in the real world,
Fletcher says.
|
24th January 2009 | | |
Researcher refutes link between video games and school shootings
| From gamepolitics.com
|
A researcher at Texas A&M International University has concluded that there is no significant relationship between school shootings and playing violent video games.
Writing for the Journal of Investigative Psychology and Offender
Profiling, Prof. Christopher Ferguson criticizes the methodology used in earlier research linking games to violence and aggression. He also points out that no evidence of violent game play was found in recent high-profile incidents such as the Virginia
Tech massacre, the Utah Trolley Stop mall shooting and the February, 2008 shooting on the campus of Northern Illinois University.
Ferguson examines the notion of moral panic as it relates to the supposed relationship between violent video
games and school shootings: Moral panics may emerge from culture wars occurring in a society... politicians, news media and social scientists, arguably [have] motives for promoting hysterical beliefs about
media violence, and video games specifically. Actual causes of violent crime, such as family environment, genetics, poverty, and inequality, are oftentimes difficult, controversial, and intractable problems. By contrast, video games present something of
a straw man by which politicians can create an appearance of taking action against crime...
Ferguson, who cites GamePolitics among his numerous sources, notes that many video game critics are unfamiliar with the medium:
It has been the observation of this author, for instance, that the majority of individuals critical of video games are above the age of 35 (many are elderly) and oftentimes admit to not having directly experienced
the games. Some commentators make claims betraying their unfamiliarity, such as that games like Grant Theft Auto ‘award points’ for antisocial behaviour... despite that few games award points for anything anymore, instead focusing on stories.
Ferguson also points out what he sees as design flaws in a number of studies relating to video games and aggression. He also examines school shooting research conducted by the FBI and Secret Service before concluding:
School shootings, although exceedingly rare, are an important issue worthy of serious consideration. However, for our understanding of this phenomenon to progress, we must move past the moral panic on video games and
other media and take a hard look at the real causes of serious aggression and violence...
the wealth of evidence... fails to establish a link between violent video games and violent crimes, including school shootings. The link has not merely been
unproven; I argue that the wealth of available data simply weighs against any causal relationship.
|
19th January 2009 | | |
Games research suggests that violence is not necessary for fun
| From gamepolitics.com See also Abstract to The
Motivating Role of Violence in Video Games
|
While video games are often slammed over violent content, a new study suggests that it is the challenge presented by a game rather than graphic violence which attracts players.
The research, which appears in the Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin , was conducted at the University of Rochester.
A press release quotes Andrew Przybylski, the study's lead author: For the vast majority of players, even those who regularly play and enjoy violent games, violence was
not a plus. Violent content was only preferred by a small subgroup of people that generally report being more aggressive.
Immersyve president Scott Rigby commented on potential ramifications for the video game industry: Much of the debate
about game violence has pitted the assumed commercial value of violence against social concern about the harm it may cause. Our study shows that the violence may not be the real value component, freeing developers to design away from violence while at
the same time broadening their market.
Researchers incorporated the popular Half-Life 2 and House of the Dead III into their study, using both high and low gore scenarios.
|
28th November 2008 | | |
German nutters and politicians discuss violent computer games
| From gamepolitics.com by Soldat Louis |
German nutters and politicians have just held a conference on violent computer games: Douglas Gentile was, by far, the most moderate of the panel. He called to get rid of the simplistic idea that video games are either
good or bad. And although he criticized ESRB, he opposed to a ban of the most violent games, asking for more media literacy instead.
Werner Hopf, who presented a longitudinal
study claiming that violent video games is the most important risk factor in violent criminality rejected this idea, claiming that it
was a trick of video game industry. Not only did he call for a ban of extremely violent computer games, but he also called for the suppression of USK (German rating systems) because according to him it's too close to the industry. He asked for its
replacement by a more independent rating organization. [Hopf's study found that (1) playing violent electronic games is the strongest risk factor of violent criminality and (2) both media-stimulated and real experiences of aggressive
emotions associated with the motive of revenge are core risk factors of violence in school and violent criminality. The results of our study show that the more frequently children view horror and violence films during childhood and the more frequently
they play violent electronic games at the beginning of adolescence the higher will these students' violence and delinquency be at the age of 14].
USK was also criticized by researchers from the KFN, the Criminology Institute
lead by Christian Pfeiffer, one of the most vocal German opponents against killer games . Regine Pfeiffer, Christian's sister, even attacked Electronic Arts violently, calling it a pig company. [According to the report, she was
frustrated in her efforts to sue EA over a violent game (Dead Space?) because the publisher is not headquartered in Germany]. Finally, journalist Rainer Fromm reiterated his objections against sadistic and militaristic games.
But he also said that he considered video games per se as a great hobby, even telling that he plays them regularly as well as his children. He also reiterated his very positive opinion of eSports.
Bavarian Interior Minister Joachim Hermann was
happy about the success of this conference, and it confirmed him in his view that some violent games such as GTA 4 or The Godfather : Don Edition must be banned...
|
17th November 2008 | |
| Researchers find that violent games may effect sleep
| Adrenalin anybody? Based on article from gamepolitics.com
|
New research from Sweden indicates that violent video games affect boys' heart rate and sleep, according to Science Daily.
The study, conducted by researchers from Stockholm University, Uppsala University and Karolinska Institute, tracked
12-15-year-old boys who were asked to play two different games: The heart rate variability was affected to a higher degree when the boys were playing games focusing on violence compared with games without
violent features. Differences in heart rate variability were registered both while the boys were playing the games and when they were sleeping that night. The boys themselves did not feel that they had slept poorly after having played violent games.
The results show that the autonomous nerve system, and thereby central physiological systems in the body, can be affected when you play violent games without your being aware of it. It is too early to draw conclusions about what the long-term
significance of this sort of influence might be. What is important about this study is that the researchers have found a way, on the one hand, to study what happens physiologically when you play video or computer games and, on the other hand, to discern
the effects of various types of games.
The researchers hope that their work may also have some implications for the study of so-called game addiction.
|
4th November 2008 | | |
Research suggests that violent games lead to getting into more trouble at school
| Based on
article from washingtonpost.com |
Children and teenagers who play violent video games show increased physical aggression months afterward, according to new research.
The research, published today in the journal Pediatrics , brings together three studies, one from the
United States and two from Japan, examining the content of games, how often they are played and aggressive behaviors later in a school year.
The U.S. research looked at the effects of violent video games over time, said lead author Craig A.
Anderson, a psychology professor at Iowa State University and director of its Center for the Study of Violence.
Anderson said the collaboration with Japanese researchers was particularly telling because video games are popular there and crime and
aggression are less prevalent. Some gamers have cited Japan's example as evidence that violent games are not harmful.
Yet the studies produced similar findings in both countries, Anderson said. When you find consistent effects across two very
different cultures, you're looking at a pretty powerful phenomenon. One can no longer claim this is somehow a uniquely American phenomenon. This is a general phenomenon that occurs across cultures.
The study in the United States showed an
increased likelihood of getting into a fight at school or being identified by a teacher or peer as being physically aggressive five to six months later in the same school year. It focused on 364 children ages 9 to 12 in Minnesota and was first included
in a 2007 book, Violent Video Game Effects on Children and Adolescents.
Japanese researchers studied more than 1,200 Japanese youths ages 12 to 18. In all three studies, researchers accounted for gender and previous aggressiveness.
The American Academy of Pediatrics, which publishes the journal in which the study appears this month, is in the process of revising its recommendations on media violence, and expects to issue a new statement in four to six months, a spokeswoman said.
The academy now recognizes violence in media as a significant health risk to children and adolescents and recommends limiting screen time including television, computers and video games to one to two hours a day. Refuted
Based on article from gamepolitics.com In a letter to Pediatrics , Christopher Ferguson, a researcher at Texas A&M International University, has called the Anderson study into question. Ferguson claims that
the research contains numerous flaws and disputes its meaningfulness. Ferguson writes:
In the literature review the authors suggest that research on video game violence is consistent when this is hardly the case. The
authors here simply ignore a wide body of research which conflicts with their views...
The authors fail to control for relevant "third" variables that could easily explain the weak correlations that they find. Family violence exposure
for instance, peer group influences, certainly genetic influences on aggressive behavior are just a few relevant variables that ought either be controlled or at minimum acknowledged as alternate causal agents for (very small) link between video games and
aggression...
Lastly the authors link their results to youth violence in ways that are misleading and irresponsible. The authors do not measure youth violence in their study. The [research tool used] is not a violence measure, nor does it even
measure pathological aggression. Rather this measure asks for hypothetical responses to potential aggressive situations, not actual aggressive behaviors.
|
17th September 2008 | | |
US games survey finds a broadly neutral effect on civic life
| Based on article from gamepolitics.com See also
Teens, Video Games & Civics [pdf] from pewinternet.org
See Computer games positive for kids from whyhealthy.blogspot.com
|
The Pew Internet & American Life Project has just released the results of the first-ever US, publicly available look at youth and video games.
Teens, Video Games & Civics examines how and why games are played and details the
relationship that gaming has to social and civic engagement among teens in the United States.
In gathering their data, Pew conducted phone interviews with 12-17-year olds along with a parent. The results of the 75-page report are a fascinating
glimpse into how video games fit into the lives of teens. Major conclusions include:
- Almost all teens play games
- 90% of parents say they always or sometimes know what games their children play.
- 72% say they always or sometimes check the ratings before their children are allowed to play a game.
- Parents of
teens who play games are generally neutral on the effect of games on their children, with nearly two-thirds believing that games have no impact one way or the other on their offspring.
- 62% of parents of gamers say video games have no effect on
their child one way or the other.
- 19% of parents of gamers say video games have a positive influence on their child
- 13% of parents of gamers say video games have a negative influence on their child.
- 5% of parents of gamers say
gaming has some negative influence/some positive influence, but it depends on the game.
Civic engagement was one of the main focal points of the study. Games, however, seemed to have a mostly neutral effect in this area, with much depending on the civic-mindedness of individual gamers:
|
21st May 2008 | |
| Why isn't there an epidemic of violence?
| See full article from
Science Daily
|
Does playing violent video games make players aggressive? It is a question that has taxed researchers, sociologists, and regulators ever since the first console was plugged into a TV and the first shots fired in a shoot 'em up game.
Writing
May 14 in the International Journal of Liability and Scientific Enquiry, Patrick Kierkegaard of the University of Essex, England, suggests that there is scant scientific evidence that video games are anything but harmless and that they do not lead to
real world aggression. Moreover, his research shows that previous work is biased towards the opposite conclusion.
Kierkegaard points out that violent games are growing more realistic with each passing year and most relish their plots of violence,
aggression and gender bias. But, he asks, Is there any scientific evidence to support the claims that violent games contribute to aggressive and violent behaviour?
Media scare stories about gamers obsessed with violent games and many
research reports that claim to back up the idea that virtual violence breeds real violence would seem to suggest so. However, Kierkegaard has studied a range of such research papers several of which have concluded since the early 1980s that video games
can lead to juvenile delinquency, fighting at school and during free play periods and violent criminal behaviour such as assault and robbery.
However, Kierkegaard explains, there is no obvious link between real-world violence statistics and the
advent of video games. If anything, the effect seems to be the exact opposite and one might argue that video game usage has reduced real violence. Despite several high profile incidents in US academic institutions, Violent crime, particularly among
the young, has decreased dramatically since the early 1990s, says Kierkegaard: while video games have steadily increased in popularity and use. For example, in 2005, there were 1,360,088 violent crimes reported in the USA compared with 1,423,677
the year before. With millions of sales of violent games, the world should be seeing an epidemic of violence, instead, violence has declined.
Research is inconclusive, emphasises Kierkegaard. It is possible that certain types of video
game could affect emotions, views, behaviour, and attitudes, however, so can books, which can lead to violent behaviour on those already predisposed to violence. The inherent biases in many of the research studies examined by Kierkegaard point to a need
for a more detailed study of video games and their psychological effects.
|
8th April 2008 | | |
Research suggests that violent video games calm people down
| See full article from
Game Daily
|
A new study in the UK has found that playing online violent games actually reduces anger and relaxes gamers.
While some anti-gaming activists would love nothing more than to find new research that definitively links real life violence with
violence in video games, one recent study in Britain found that playing violent video games online actually has a tendency to make people less angry. Miss Jane Barnett and her colleagues at Middlesex University are presented their results at the
British Psychological Society's Annual Conference in Dublin.
For the study, 292 male and female World of Warcraft players, aged between 12 and 83, were given a questionnaire on anger, aggression and personality. The participants then played the
game for two hours and then completed the survey yet again. Ultimately, the results showed that the gamers were more likely to feel calm or tired after playing – but there were differences depending on sex, age and personality.
There
were actually higher levels of relaxation before and after playing the game as opposed to experiencing anger but this did very much depend on personality type. This will help us to develop a emotion and gaming questionnaire to help distinguish the type
of gamer who is likely to transfer their online aggression into everyday life, explained Barnett.
|
7th April 2008 | | |
Computer game addicts like people with Asperger's
| See full article from
University of Bolton
|
People who are addicted to playing computer games show some of the same personality traits as people with Asperger's syndrome.
This is the conclusion of Dr John Charlton of the University of Bolton and Ian Danforth of Whitman College, USA.
Their results were presented at the British Psychological Society's Annual Conference in Dublin,
The researchers questioned 391 computer game players, 86% of whom were male. They considered relationships between addiction, 'high engagement' and
personality.
They found that the closer the players got to addiction the more likely they were to display negative personality traits. And that as players showed more signs of addiction they were increasingly characterised by three personality
traits that would normally be associated with Asperger's, a variety of high functioning autism. These were neuroticism, and lack of extraversion and agreeableness.
The researchers believe that these people are not classifiable as having Aspergers
syndrome but share some of the same characteristics because they find it easier to empathise with computer systems than other people.
Dr Charlton said: 'The thinking in the field is that there is a scale along which people, even those
considered to be 'normal', can be placed upon. And that people such as engineers, mathematicians and computer scientists are nearer to the non-empathising, systemising, end of the spectrum, with people with Asperger's syndrome even further along again.
Our research supports the idea that people who are heavily involved in game playing may be nearer to autistic spectrum disorders than people who have no interest in gaming. Update:
Poor Communications 8th April 2008 Dr John Charlton, a psychologist from the University of Bolton, has clarified that gaming does not cause Asperger Syndrome. Dr Charlton
pointed out that some media outlets have been misinterpreting his research into video game addiction, which noted that some addicted gamers exhibit characteristics similar to those of people with Asperger Syndrome.
In no way can it be said
that Asperger's can be caused by game playing (Asperger's is thought to have a biological basis) , SPOnG was told.
|
6th March 2008 | | |
|
The surprising truth about violent video games See article from gamecouch.com |
2nd March 2008 | | |
Do we really enjoy the deaths of our digital opponents?
| See full article from
ShackNews See also study: The Psychophysiology of James Bond |
DA new study by a team of researchers from Helsinki, Finland has made the news lately due to its tentative answer.
The team's surprising findings: Video game players from its test group were angered and anxious after shooting their enemies,
while their own deaths elicited a response of joy.
The study: The Psychophysiology of James Bond: Phasic Emotional Responses to Violent Video Game Events recorded the psychophysiological responses of 36 young adults while playing James
Bond 007: NightFire .
Previous studies have postulated that pairing violent actions with positive rewards is a recipe for desensitization. Though the Helsinki researchers expected their subjects to enjoy the act of killing, the subjects'
responses were just the opposite.
The Bond researchers tentatively concluded that their results, which showed a negative response to the death of an enemy, lend little credence to the idea that video games desensitize players to violence: From
this perspective, the fact that wounding or killing the opponent elicited negative, not positive, emotional responses might be reassuring .
With that hypothesis in mind, the team synced recorded game video with physiological data gathered
from the players. In some cases, data from a non-violent event in Super Monkey Ball 2 , picking a banana, was used as a comparison to the violent Bond game.
The data showed that not only do players react negatively to the deaths of
opponents, but they also react positively to the death or wounding of their own character. However, rather than concluding that video game players are sadistic individuals, the team is guessing that players are simply relieved from the tension of
playing.
Though the brief relief from combat may explain the positive response to a player's own death, an enemy death is also followed by a reprieve from combat, making the negative response all the more interesting.
The study also
showed that even after multiple play sessions, users still responded to the events in much the same way.
There was no evidence for desensitization of emotional responses as a function of repeated exposures to violent game events, concluded
the study.
|
31st December 2007 | | |
Games cause decreased blood flow to the brain
| From Taiwan Headlines |
People who spend too much time playing video games, especially violent video games, may risk damaging brain functions and affect their learning and emotional control, according to findings of a study.
Chou Yuan-hua, a doctor in the Department of
Psychiatry of Taipei Veterans General Hospital, enlisted 30 youngsters, all aged 25, as research subjects.
They were given physical examinations to monitor changes in blood circulation in their brains before and after each played a video game for
30 minutes.
The study found that the act of playing video games obviously causes a decreased blood flow in the brain, and that the effect is even more pronounced in those playing violent video games.
Noting that the study focused on
subjects who played video games for only 30 minutes, Chou said many youngsters spend far more time on video games each day, unaware that doing so on a long term basis could damage the frontal lobe of the brain, as well as the anterior cingulate gyrus.
He explained that the frontal lobe of the cerebral cortex, located at the front of the brain, is associated with thinking, speaking, decision-making and impulse control, while the anterior cingulate gyrus, located in the medial wall of the
frontal lobe, modulates internal emotional responses.
Although no conclusions as yet have been drawn regarding what might happen if blood flow to these brain areas is diminished frequently, it is already known that frontal lobe damage can leave a
person emotionally adrift, opening him or her up to odd mood changes and variations in social behavior and personality.
Chou added that clinical experiences have proven that patients with schizophrenia or depression have significantly lower blood
flows in their frontal lobes and anterior cingulate gyruses.
|
|
Video games found to be teaching life skills to gamers
|
|
|
| 30th
November 2007
|
|
| |
Violent Video Games as Exemplary Teachers: A Conceptual Analysis. By
- Douglas A. Gentile, Institute of Science and Society Iowa State University National Institute on Media and the Family
- J. Ronald Gentile, University at Buffalo, State University of New York
Abstract This article presents conceptual and empirical analyses of several of the “best practices” of learning and instruction, and demonstrates how violent video games use them effectively to motivate
learners to persevere in acquiring and mastering a number of skills, to navigate through complex problems and changing environments, and to experiment with different identities until success is achieved. These educational
principles allow for the generation of several testable hypotheses, two of which are tested with samples of 430 elementary school children (mean age 10 years), 607 young adolescents (mean age 14 years), and 1,441 older adolescents (mean age 19 years).
Participants were surveyed about their video game habits and their aggressive cognitions and behaviours. The first hypothesis is based on the principle that curricula that teach the same underlying concepts across contexts should
have the highest transfer. Therefore, students who play multiple violent video games should be more likely to learn aggressive cognitions and behaviours than those who play fewer. The second hypothesis is based on the principle
that long-term learning is improved if practice is distributed more across time. Therefore, students who play violent video games more frequently across time should be more likely to learn aggressive cognitions and behaviours than those who play the same
types of games for equivalent amounts of time but less frequently. Both hypotheses were supported. We conclude by describing what educators can learn from the successful instructional and curriculum design features of video games.
|
|
|